column-gray-032715

Introduction: What Moves Us

Since I started the articles about “motivation” my goal was to answer the question: “What puts us in motion?”

The word “motivate” is etymologically derived from the Latin word motus, the past participle of movere (to move), origin of Medieval Latin motivus and the Old French motif (moving, will) and was added to the verb-forming suffix -ate. The current scientific understanding of motivation is closer to the etymological origin or the word than to its popular modern use. It means to “set in willful motion.

The next step is to explore the “willful” part of the concept. What are the behavioral aspects of internally creating motion?


RECENT: The Role of Grit in Sport Performance


In this article, our journey will be along the thread that weaves together the concepts of self-regulation, self-control, self-efficacy, and self-motivation. All of them refer, in a way, to the idea of control, as opposed to being controlled. One must be in control or have autonomy, to set goals, move through intrinsic and extrinsic motivators, and grittily, hardily, toughly keep on track. That is true for all long-term activities involving performance and learning, with sport being one of the most rigorously determined by these related cognitive-affective phenomena.

Self-Control and Self-Regulation

This story starts with something called “executive function.” Executive function is what the expression says: the function of deciding to execute something. It is one of the neurological and behavioral traits that characterize modern humans (Coolidge and Wynn 2001).

Executive function is not a single function but rather a set of neurologically determined cognitive processes through which the individual may exert control over his own behavior in order to optimize the achievement of a goal. That includes selecting, monitoring, and facilitating some behaviors while inhibiting others. Repressing an impulse, for example, is an inhibitory control over behavior.

You may ask, “Don’t we always decide how to act?” Not really. The executive system is mobilized to act according to real-time decision-making, rather than an automatic or prepotent response. A prepotent response is one that the individual is ready to provide (Harriet 2009), triggered automatically by a stimulus (Diamond 2013, Hofmann et al 2009, Plutchik 2001). The executive system overrides the prepotent response. The prepotent response may be an impulse or a conditioned response.

Screenshot 2018-12-14 11.31.50

An impulse is a response to a specific activation stimulus from the environment with a strong incentive of immediate gratification. Impulses are strong neurologically driven responses for a reason: In order to survive, animals need to have hard-wired, fast responses to physiological needs, threats, and other survival demands. For example, the impulse to drink water when one is thirsty or the impulse to hide during a shooting. Impulses are driven by hedonistic (pleasure) or negative drives (fear, anger).

Screenshot 2018-12-14 11.34.11

From Plutchik (2001)

This is all fine if we just need to survive on the short-term. The pursuit of long-term goals, though, requires inhibiting impulses. That’s where the executive system determines or modulates action. The executive system is a set, rather than one cognitive function, associated with different brain activities (Barber & Carter 2004).

There is still no agreement about which executive functions constitute executive control (EC or the executive system), but there is a general consensus (Miyake & Friedman 2012, Friedman et al 2008) on the following:

  • Executive functions are a collection of related but separate abilities;
  • The three most studied and less controversial are prepotent response inhibition, updating working memory, and task shifting;
  • They show individual variation in the human population;
  • They are predominantly genetically determined;
  • While EC individual variability is genetically determined, the EC/ES’s development is socially influenced. As with most traits, in each stage of life, it is the result of the interplay of nature and nurture. Socioeconomic origin is critical, but executive functions can be trained (Sarsour et al 2011, Thorell et al 2009, Diamond 2012, Diamond & Ling 2016).

The Nature of Self-Control

Self-control is one aspect of the inhibitory control of prepotent responses.

There is no consensus as to how exactly self-control is exerted. The strength model of self-control offers a robust set of empirical evidence. Baumester and collaborators (2007) have defined self-control as the capacity to override temptations, or impulses that are not consistent with a given goal or set of values. His group observed early in the 1990s that self-control seemed to be vulnerable to deterioration over time, after repeated exertions, much like physical fatigue. It was conceptualized as a “limited resource” and its exhaustion, “ego depletion.” Experiments with a group challenged by a self-regulation demanding task (resisting eating chocolate, for example) and an unchallenged group showed that, with time, the challenged group loses performance.

Screenshot 2018-12-14 11.40.44

From Baumeister et al. 2007

Several studies have suggested that there might be depletion-mitigating procedures or situations, such as humor, alternative cognitive approaches, and certain cultural resources.

limited resource effect

From Baumeister et al. 2007

Like in sports, ego-depletion fatigue is temporary (Evans et al 2016).

One approach to reducing ego-depletion is to decrease the task effort by introducing automatic responses (Wieber & Gollwitzer 2016). We saw that one of the purposes of executive function was to provide tools for decision-making under uncertainty. That can only be accomplished by overriding prepotent responses and creating, through willful effort, a new strategy. Depletion is a result of exerting effort faster than recovery can replenish the resource.

Think about the learning stage of a sport-specific motor skill: It is exhausting and clumsy because most tasks are still not automatic.

Controversy still exists, but the ego-depletion model stands strong (Dang 2018).

Regardless of whether or not self-control is a limited resource, the lay belief that willpower is limited seems to be detrimental. While researchers have access to the variables involved when they study self-regulation demanding tasks, the subjects don’t. If they assume their willpower will be exhausted sooner or later, their performance will be poorer than if they assume their willpower is unlimited (Bernecker 2016).

The harder the task, the more detrimental the limited willpower theory will be to the performer:

Screenshot 2018-12-14 11.45.20

From Bernecker 2016

This is relevant to coaching strategies and athlete management.

Self-Efficacy: “Know Thyself”

memento mori

Memento Mori mosaic from excavations in the convent of San Gregorio, Via Appia, Rome, Italy. Now in the National Museum Bath of Diocletian, Rome, Italy. The Greek motto gnōthi sauton (know thyself, nosce te ipsum) combines with the image to convey the famous warning: Respice post te; hominem te esse memento; memento mori. (Look behind; remember that you are mortal; remember death.) – public domain (The artist is unknown. - Lessing Photo Archive: http://www.lessing-photo.com/p3/110103/11010329.jpg)

Albert Bandura (1991) explained that self-efficacy is a part of self-regulatory behavior. Through the beliefs people form about what they can do, they may exercise forethought and motivate themselves in an anticipatory proactive way. That is how personal agency plays a determinant role in purposeful behavior.

Screenshot 2018-12-14 11.51.52

From Bandura (1991)

To form a self-efficacy representation, a person necessarily must observe and monitor himself or herself and elaborate an ongoing diagnostic. In well-organized activities such as academics or sports, people have the tools to accurately observe themselves through quantitative measures. Goal-setting and self-monitoring feedback into one another; there is no observation and diagnosis in the void. You can only observe, monitor, and measure your performance in a specific context with specific standards. How fast are you? How fast are you relative to your goal’s standards? What is your deadlift PR today? What is the (world/regional) record? In several activities, these standards are externally determined, making it easier to conduct precise observations. The goal itself, though, is wider than meeting a standard.

Self-observation enhances performance (motivation in action) when there is clear evidence of progress and clear goals. In this scenario, the higher the goals, the more the performance increases:

Screenshot 2018-12-14 11.53.14

Realistic self-monitoring with high self-efficacy is correlated to success. Self-monitoring that over-emphasizes failure, however, produces negative performance results. Unrealistic positive self-diagnosis is catastrophic for the same reasons.

Screenshot 2018-12-14 11.54.02

Self-Control and Self-Regulation in Sports and Exercise

Self-control and self-regulation in sports are associated with the maintenance of motivation and goal structure, which organize and drive successful performance. Studies show that regular strength and aerobic training (necessary components of sports training) stimulate executive function, therefore, self-control and self-regulation. There seems to be a measurable cognitive improvement after acute exercise and sports tasks that require self-regulatory functions (Brush et al 2016, Heath et al 2018). Is this carry-over instead of depletion?

The opposite – experimental self-control exhaustion on a cognitive task and its effect over exercise – seems to confirm the depletion hypothesis (Boat et al 2018).

Few studies have measured the effects of elite sports training over self-regulation on other types of tasks (Diamond 2015).

The principles of self-regulation within an executive control approach hold, of course, but we don’t know how sports self-regulatory tasks interact with other life tasks.

Dangerous Interactions: Integrated Self-Regulation

As much as the virtuous cycle of self-control and self-regulation in exercise tend to optimize health and well-being, requiring less investment, the loss of self-control in one key area of life, has a contagion or trickling down effect over others.

One interesting hint at this is that things that affect executive function, like sleep deprivation, have a detrimental effect over all self-regulated behaviors. Taking the sleep deprivation example:

  • Executive function indicators show a decrease.
  • Self-control over food cravings becomes much harder as cortisol levels rise, leptin declines, ghrelin production increases, and cognitive functions in general decrease.
  • There is generalized cognitive function decrease. That also means they require more and more effort.
  • Several physiological functions are impaired, academic production is affected as well as emotional control.
  • Inter-personal relations and functions mediating them degenerate.
  • If chronic, sleep deprivation is associated with depression, substance abuse, and suicide.

Certain substances lead to disinhibition. Disinhibition feels good (hedonic experience) and may lead to more immediate gratification behaviors at the cost of self-regulated behavior. Studies suggest that substance abuse may be both a cause and a consequence of impulsivity (De Wit 2009).

Is it possible to achieve and maintain:

  • Elite performance levels in sports;
  • Exceptional academic performance;
  • Overall health reflected in self-regulated eating behavior, regular sleep, sex, and mental habits;
  • And healthy interpersonal relationships.

while failing to develop self-control over another area of life? For example:

  • Can an elite athlete be a gambler?
  • Can a world-class scientist be an alcoholic?
  • Can a highly productive and talented artist have poor eating, sleep, and relationship behaviors?

Yes, they can, and these cases make the news, leading people to believe that they are the rule. They are not. These cases are usually associated with a prevalent role of talent over cognitive or personality traits that require effort to maintain. Eventually, the deficit in executive function may catch up with the rest of the individual’s behavior. It can also accelerate death, so they die as extremely successful in certain areas of their lives while a total mess in others.

I am not aware of studies highlighting the interaction between self-regulatory behaviors. Depletion may explain extreme cases: Self-regulatory resources are allocated to certain tasks, and there are none left for others.

The takeaway from all this is that the construction and maintenance of a sturdy motivational architecture requires effort. Even if it is possible (or necessary) to sacrifice self-control over certain behaviors to excel in others, there requires a great deal of compartmentalization.

If your choice is to be or continue being a successful high-performance athlete, though, it is a good idea to accept that true, scientifically defined motivation is in part a gift from mom and dad but requires your willful effort to keep it that way.

The Takeaways

  • Executive functions are an inherited trait.
  • Life will present uncertainty and disruptive experiences. How many and to what extent they will impact you is unpredictable. Your executive functions are the cognitive tools to manage uncertainty.
  • As a corollary to that, your athletic career also contains uncertainty. Without self-regulatory responses, specifically self-control, your career might become catastrophic.
  • The more you educate yourself, the better equipped you are to self-regulate your behavior.
  • Self-control, when successful, generates more self-control with less effort.
  • Don’t overdo. Exert self-control within reasonable limits.
  • Self-monitor with special attention to successful results. Use failures as a learning tool.
  • Avoid engaging in potentially dangerous and life-disruptive impulsive behaviors. Use impulsivity with moderation.
  • Think probabilistically: Life will probably be a long-term experiment. If your sport plays a central role in your life, you need to have goals and a plan. You must self-regulate your behavior.

References

  1. Bandura, Albert. "Social cognitive theory of self-regulation." Organizational behavior and human decision processes 50, no. 2 (1991): 248-287.
  2. Barber, Anita D., and Cameron S. Carter. "Cognitive control involved in overcoming prepotent response tendencies and switching between tasks." Cerebral Cortex 15, no. 7 (2004): 899-912.
  3. Baumeister, Roy F., Kathleen D. Vohs, and Dianne M. Tice. "The strength model of self-control." Current directions in psychological science 16, no. 6 (2007): 351-355.
  4. Bernecker, Katharina. Implicit Theories About Willpower and Their Consequences for Achievement, Health, and Well-Being. 2016, Ph.D. diss., University of Zurich, Faculty of Arts.
  5. Boat, Ruth, Todd Atkins, Nathan Davenport, and Simon Cooper. "Prior self-control exertion and perceptions of pain and motivation during a physically effortful task." Progress in brain research 240 (2018): 19-34.
  6. Brush, Christopher J., Ryan L. Olson, Peter J. Ehmann, Steven Osovsky, and Brandon L. Alderman. "Dose-response and time course effects of acute resistance exercise on executive function." Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology38, no. 4 (2016): 396-408.
  7. Coolidge, Frederick L., and Thomas Wynn. "Executive functions of the frontal lobes and the evolutionary ascendancy of Homo sapiens." Cambridge archaeological journal 11, no. 2 (2001): 255-260.
  8. Dang, Junhua. "Can the ego be depleted?: Attempts to replicate the ego-depletion effect and integrate its explanations." Ph.D. diss, Lund University, 2018.
  9. De Wit, Harriet. "Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes." Addiction biology 14, no. 1 (2009): 22-31.
  10. Diamond, Adele, and Daphne S. Ling. "Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not." Developmental cognitive neuroscience 18 (2016): 34-48.
  11. Diamond, Adele. "Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions." Current directions in psychological science 21, no. 5 (2012): 335-341.
  12. Diamond, Adele. "Effects of physical exercise on executive functions: going beyond simply moving to moving with thought." Annals of sports medicine and research 2, no. 1 (2015): 1011.
  13. Diamond, Adele. "Executive functions." Annual review of psychology 64 (2013): 135-168.
  14. Evans, Daniel R., Ian A. Boggero, and Suzanne C. Segerstrom. "The nature of self-regulatory fatigue and “ego depletion” lessons from physical fatigue." Personality and Social Psychology Review 20, no. 4 (2016): 291-310.
  15. Friedman, Naomi P., Akira Miyake, Susan E. Young, John C. DeFries, Robin P. Corley, and John K. Hewitt. "Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin." Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 137, no. 2 (2008): 201.
  16. Heath, Matthew, Andrea Petrella, Jonathan Blazevic, David Lim, Andre Pelletier, and Glen R. Belfry. "A post-exercise facilitation of executive function is independent of aerobically supported metabolic costs." Neuropsychologia 120 (2018): 65-74.
  17. Hofmann, Wilhelm, Malte Friese, and Fritz Strack. "Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective." Perspectives on Psychological Science 4, no. 2 (2009): 162-176.
  18. Miyake, Akira, and Naomi P. Friedman. "The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions." Current directions in psychological science 21, no. 1 (2012): 8-14.
  19. Plutchik, Robert. "The nature of emotions: Human emotions have deep evolutionary roots, a fact that may explain their complexity and provide tools for clinical practice." American Scientist 89, no. 4 (2001): 344-350.
  20. Sarsour, Khaled, Margaret Sheridan, Douglas Jutte, Amani Nuru-Jeter, Stephen Hinshaw, and W. Thomas Boyce. "Family socioeconomic status and child executive functions: The roles of language, home environment, and single parenthood." Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 17, no. 1 (2011): 120-132.
  21. Thorell, Lisa B., Sofia Lindqvist, Sissela Bergman Nutley, Gunilla Bohlin, and Torkel Klingberg. "Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children." Developmental Science 12, no. 1 (2009): 106-113.
  22. Wieber, Frank, and Peter M. Gollwitzer. "Decoupling goal striving from resource depletion by forming implementation intentions." In Self-Regulation and Ego Control, pp. 43-65. 2016.

Header image courtesy of Wavebreak Media Ltd © 123rf.com

The Role of Mental Toughness in Sport Performance

bar-shield-10-home-r (1)